G, then technique t to group G), and a sharp disagreement between the
social groups - not the same technique, not the same finished products,
not the same social group – as seen in Ethiopia, Cameroon and India.
This disagreement promotes differentiation while, at the same time,
affiliation is reinforced in the course of learning, communities of
practice and cultural process of identity construction shaping each
other (
Lave, 1991, p. 80).
Interactions are of a major importance in the polarization process.
Indeed, interactions are required for actors to correlate techniques and
social groups as shown by Indian teenagers. These interactions can be of
different nature, as exemplified by the four case studies, but they al-
ways take place between actors who live in close geographical proxi-
mity and who in this regard are strongly connected.
3.3. Conditions for persistence of technological boundaries
The four case studies show that independently of the organization of
production, interactions between actors belonging to different well
connected groups are not sufficient for positive influence when the
differences between the groups are higher than within the groups. The
technological standards are strong markers of differences, creating
inter-group disagreement (even within a same social group). As a
consequence, these technological standards contribute directly to en-
hance and add differentiation between interacting communities, espe-
cially if those belong to different social groups. It explains that persis-
tence of technological boundaries has been systematically observed in
situations where different communities live in close geographical
proximity and use different technological standards. Whatever the
cultural context and the local social situation, these interactions have
promoted negative influence.
Lastly, let us note that there may be exceptions to this regularity.
One example takes place in Cameroon where one Mambila potter (FAN-
Cp, whose mother SAB-Cp taught her Cp) adopted recently Md, after
visiting MAR-Md and ELH-Md and practicing with them, from the
preparation of the clay paste to the forming of the vessel. As a result,
there has been a positive influence and an integrative effect. This po-
sitive influence reveals:
(a) The role of the cognitive bias correlating here clay material and
fashioning technique; the potter did not borrow only the forming
technique, but also the clay material, linking both features which,
in the case of Cameroon, appear to have played a role in the dif-
ferentiation process between the four groups, each of them using
different clay materials and recipes.
(b) The role of social learning. Borrowing new practices has implied
social learning between individuals from the Md and Cp social
groups through common practice, from the preparation of the clay
until the forming of the pot. In a situation of technological polar-
ization, such a situation is rare. It goes with exceptional close re-
lationships. This has been observed in Cameroon, and in India, in
the town of Jahangirabad (Bulandshar dist.) where a Hindu potter
and a Muslim potter had developed exceptional friendship followed
by a unique case of borrowing of the kiln by the Hindu potter.
4. Conclusions
Field studies have enabled us to test empirically the predictions of
the experimental model elaborated by
Flache and Macy (2011)
while
examining the
micro-processes at play in the non-borrowing of tech-
niques. They highlight that interactions between groups using different
technological standards and living in close geographical proximity re-
present conditions in favor of the persistence of technological bound-
aries. Technological standards add to differentiation between groups
due to a cognitive bias developed at the individual level. Polarization
along with negative influence increases when the technologically
marked groups belong to different social groups. The former do not
correspond necessarily to distinctive, bounded, homogeneous social
groups; their nature can be expressed by their degree of “groupness”,a
term coined by Brubaker and Cooper while debating identity (Brubaker
and Cooper, 2000, p. 20). By using the term “groupness”, the point is to
take advantage of:
“an analytical idiom sensitive to the multiple forms and degrees of
commonality and connectedness, and to the widely varying ways in
which actors (…) attribute meaning and significance to them. This
will enable us to distinguish instances of strongly binding, vehe-
mently felt groupness from more loosely structured, weakly con-
straining forms of affinity and affiliation.”
Brubaker and Cooper, 2000, p. 21
Our four case studies illustrate different degrees of groupness, from
a weakly constraining form of affinity as in Ecuador, to a strongly sense
of groupness expressed by explicit reference to the other group as in
Ethiopia, Cameroon and India. The sense of groupness is enhanced
when technological boundaries overlap social boundaries, strengthened
then by greater differentiation between groups. In archaeology con-
textual data should help to correlate technological clusters with dif-
ferent degrees of groupness (e.g. Parkinson, 2006).
Polarization and negative influence can last for centuries, as ex-
emplified by the long co-existence of wheel made and hand-made
ceramics around the Mediterranean basin (
Gauss et al., 2016).
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the ANR (The French National Agency
for Research) within the framework of the program CULT
(Metamorphosis of societies –“Emergences and evolution of cultures
and cultural phenomena”), project DIFFCERAM (Dynamics of spreading
of ceramic techniques and style: actualist comparative data and agent-
based modeling) (n°ANR-12-CULT-0001-01).
The data were collected, analyzed and illustrated: in Cameroon by
E. Zangato and G. de Saulieu; in Ecuador, by C. Lara; in Ethiopia by J.
Cauliez, C. Manen and A.-L. Goujon; in India by B. Bril and V. Roux. The
paper has been written by V. Roux.
In Cameroon, we are most grateful to the students Anselme Ossima,
François Ngouoh and Ruth Wa ffa, from the University of Yaounde 1,
and Guillaume Delebarre, from the University of Paris Nanterre, who
took part in the fieldwork during the seasons of 2013 and 2014. We
thank also the IRD Yaounde, the department of archaeology of Yaounde
1, the sub-prefect of Bankim and the traditional Tikar chief of Bankim
for their help in the fi
eld. In Ecuador, we thank Tamara Landívar,
curator of
the ethnographic department of the ethnographic museum of
Pumapungo in Cuenca, Ministry of Culture, for her help and her interest
in the project. In Ethiopia, the project was supported by the Fyssen
Foundation and the UMR 5608 – Traces (CNRS, University of Toulouse
Jean-Jaurès, France) and benefited from the help of the Authority for
Research and Conservation of Cultural Heritage (The Federal
Democratic Republic of Ethiopia, Ministry of Culture and Tourism,
Addis Ababa, Ethiopia) and the French Center of Ethiopian Studies
(IFRE 23 and USR 3137, Addis Ababa, Ethiopia). The data have been
collected with the help of Vincent Ard (CNRS, UMR 5608) and
Joséphine Caro (PhD candidate, UMR 5608). In India, the project was
supported by the UMR 7055 – Préhistoire & Technologie (CNRS,
University of Paris Nanterre, France). We thank all the potters in
Cameroon, Ecuador, Ethiopia and India for their availability and their
unfailing kindness. We thank also two anonymous reviewers for their
constructive and useful feedback.
References
Arnold, P.J.I., 1991. Domestic Ceramic Production and Spatial Organization: A Mexican
Case Study in Ethnoarchaeology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Axelrod, R., 1997. The dissemination of culture a model with local convergence and
V. Roux et al.
Jour nal of Anthropol ogic al Ar chaeol ogy 48 (2017) 320–335
334
global polarization. J. Confl. Resolut. 41, 203–226.
Berry, J.W., Poortinga, Y.H., Segall, M.H., Dasen, P.R., 2002. Acculturation and inter-
cultural relations. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 345–383
.
Blench, R., 1993. New developments in the classification of Bantu languages and their
historical implications. In: Barreteau, D., Graffenried, C.V. (Eds.), Datation et
Chronologie dans Le Bassin du Lac Tchad. ORSTOM, Paris, pp. 147–160.
Bowser, J.B., Patton, J.Q., 2008. Learning and transmission of pottery style: women’s life
histories and communities of practice in the Ecuadorian Amazon. In: Stark, M.T.,
Bowser, B.J., Horne, L. (Eds.), Cultural Transmission and Material Culture. Breaking
Down Boundaries. The University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 105–129.
Bril, B., 2002. L’apprentissage de gestes techniques: ordre de contraintes et variations
culturelles. In: Bril, B., Roux, V. (Eds.), Le Geste Technique. Réflexions
Méthodologiques et Anthropologiques, Technologies/Idéologies/Pratiques. Editions
érès, Ramonville Saint-Agne, pp. 113–150.
Bril, B., Rein, R., Nonaka, T., Wenban-Smith, F., Dietrich, G., 2010. The role of expertise
in tool use: skill differences in functional action adaptations to task constraints. J.
Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 36, 825–839.
Brubaker, R., Cooper, F., 2000. Beyond “Identity”. Theory Soc. 29, 1–47.
Carley, K.M., 2001. Learning and using new ideas: a sociocognitive perspective. In:
Casterline, J.B. (Ed.), Diffusion Processes and Fertility Transition: Selected
Perspectives. National Academy Press, Washington, D.C., pp. 179–207
.
Collar, A., Coward, F., Brughmans, T., Mills, B.J., 2015. Networks in archaeology: phe-
nomena, abstraction, representation. J. Archaeol. Method Theory 22, 1–32.
David, N., Kramer, C., 2001. Ethnoarchaeology in Action. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Dietler, M., Herbich, I., 1998. Habitus, techniques, style: an integrated approach to the
social understanding of material culture and boundaries. In: The Archaeology of
Social Boundaries. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC and London, pp.
232–269.
Dobres, M.A., 2000. Technology and Social Agency. Blackwell Publishers, Oxford.
Flache, A., Macy, M.W., 2011. Small worlds and cultural polarization. J. Math. Sociol. 35,
146–176
.
Freeman, D., Pankhurst, A., 2003. The Excluded Minorities of Ethiopia. London.
Gallay, A., 2007. The decorated marriage jars of the inner delta of the Niger (Mali): essay
of archaeological demarcation of an ethnic territory. www.thearkeotekjournal.org
1 (1).
Gauss, W., Klebinder-Gauss, G., von Rüden, C. (Eds.), 2016. The Transmission of
Technical Knowledge in the Production of Ancient Mediterranean Pottery.
Österreichisches Archäologisches Institut, Wien.
Gelbert, A., 2003. Traditions céramiques et emprunts techniques dans la vallée du fleuve
Sénégal. Editions de la Maison des sciences de l’
homme, Editions Epistèmes, Paris.
Gosselain, O.,
2002. Poteries du Cameroun méridional styles techniques et rapports à
l’identité, Monographie du CRA. CNRS Editions, Paris
.
Gosselain, O., 2000. Materializing identities: an African perspective. J. Archaeol. Method
Theory 7, 187–217.
Gosselain, O.P., 2011. Fine if I do, fine if I don’t. Dynamics of technical knowledge in Sub-
Saharan Africa. In: Roberts, B.W., Vander Linden, M. (Eds.), Investigating
Archaeological Cultures: Material Culture, Variability, and Transmission. Springer
Science+Business Media, New York, London, pp. 211–227.
Hegmon, M., 1998. Technology, style, and social practice: archaeological approaches. In:
Stark, M.T. (Ed.), The Archaeology of Social Boundaries. Smithsonian University
Press, Washington D.C., pp. 264–279.
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., 1998. The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence
of between-group differences. Evol. Hum. Behav. 19, 215–242.
Herbich, I., 1987. Learning patterns, potter interaction and ceramic style among the Luo
of Kenya. Afr. Archaeol. Rev. 5, 109–136.
Hodder, I., 1985. Boundaries as strategies: an ethnoarchaeological study. In: The
Archaeology of Frontiers and Boundaries. Academic Press, New York.
Kelley, H.H., 1973. The processes of causal attribution. Am. Psychol. 28, 107.
Kelley, H.H., 1967. Attribution theory in social psychology. In: Levine, D. (Ed.), Nebraska
Symposium on Motivation. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln.
Kramer, C., 1991. Ceramics in two Indian cities. In: Ceramic Ethnoarchaeology. The
University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 205– 230
.
Kuhn, S., 2004. Evolutionary perspectives on technology and technological change.
World Archaeol. 36, 561–570.
Latour, B., Lemonnier, P., 1994. De la préhistoire aux missiles balistiques. L’intelligence
sociale des techniques. L’intelligence sociale des techniques, Editions La découverte,
Paris.
Lave, J., 1991. Situating learning in communities of practice. Perspect. Socially Shar.
Cogn. 2, 63–82.
Lave, J., Wenger, E., 1991. Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Lemonnier, P., 1993. Technological Choices: Transformation in Material Cultures Since
the Neolithic. Routledge, London and New York.
Lemonnier, P., 1992. Elements for an Anthropology of Technology. University of
Michigan, Ann Arbor
.
Longacre, W.A., 1991. Sources of ceramic variability among the Kalinga of Northern
Luzon. In: Longacre, W.A. (Ed.), Ceramic Ethnoarchaeology. The University of
Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 95–110
.
Longacre, W.A., Xia, J., Yang, T., 2000. I want to buy a black pot. J. Archaeol. Method
Theory 7, 273–293.
Manem, S., 2008. Etude des fondements technologiques de la culture des Duffaits (âge du
Bronze moyen). PhD thesis, Paris-X, Nanterre.
Manzo, G., 2007. Variables, mechanisms, and simulations: can the three methods be
synthesized? Rev. Fr. Sociol. 48, 35–71.
Mayor, A., 2010. Ceramic traditions and ethnicity in the Niger bend, West Africa.
Ethnoarchaeology 2, 5–48
.
McElreath, R.,
Boyd, R., Richerson, P.J., 2003. Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic
markers. Curr. Anthropol. 44, 122–130.
Mesoudi, A., 2007. Using the methods of social psychology to study cultural evolution. J.
Soc. Evol. Cult. Psychol. 1, 35–58.
Moscovici, S., 1984. Psychologie sociale. Presses Universitaires de France, Paris.
O’Brien, M.J., Bentley, R.A., 2011. Stimulated variation and cascades: two processes in
the evolution of complex technological systems. J. Archaeol. Method Theory 18,
309–337
.
Parkinson, W.A., 2006. Tribal Boundaries: stylistic variability and social boundary
maintenance during the transition to the Copper Age on the Great Hungarian Plain. J.
Anthropol. Archaeol. 25, 33–58.
Pool, C.A., 2000. Why a kiln? Firing technology in the Sierra de Los Tuxtlas, Vera Cruz
(Mexico). Archaeometry 42, 61–76
.
Rice, P.M., 1984. Pots and Potters. Current Approaches in Ceramic Archaeology. Institute
of Archaeology, University of California, Los Angeles.
Roux, V., 2015. Standardization of ceramic assemblages: transmission mechanisms and
diffusion of morpho-functional traits across social boundaries. J. Anthropol.
Archaeol. 40, 1–9.
Roux, V., 2013. Spreading of innovative technical traits and cumulative technical evo-
lution: continuity or discontinuity? J. Archaeol. Method Theory 20, 312–330.
Roux, V., Gabriellini, S., 2016. Structures de cuisson et période de transition au Rajasthan
(Inde, 2005–2015). Des choix instables avant sélection définitive. In: Müller, C.,
Heintz, M. (Eds.), Transitions Historiques. De Boccard, Paris, pp. 157–168.
Roux, V., Jeffra, C., 2016. The spreading of the potter’s wheel in the ancient
Mediterranean. A social context-dependent phenomenon. In: Gauß, W., Klebinder-
Gauß, G., von Rüden, C. (Eds.), The Transmission of Technical Knowledge in the
Production of Ancient Mediterranean Pottery. The Austrian Archaeological Institute,
Vienna, pp. 165–182
.
Roux, V., Lara, C., 2016. Why potters have not borrowed the kiln? Comparing Narratives
of Indian and Ecuadorian potters. Americae <
http://www.mae.u-paris10.fr/articles-
articulos/why-potters-have-not-borrowed-the-kiln-comparing-narratives-of-indian-
and-ecuadorian-potters/ >.
Shennan, S., 2013. Lineages of cultural transmission. In: Roy, E., Lycett, S.J., Johns, S.E.
(Eds.), Understanding Cultural Transmission in Anthropology: A Critical Synthesis,
Methodology and History in Anthropology. Berghahn Books, Oxford, pp. 346–360.
Shennan, S., Steele, J., 1999. Cultural learning in hominids: a behavioural ecological
approach. In: Box, H., Gibson, K. (Eds.), Mammalian Social Learning. Symposia of the
Zoological Society of London 70. CUP, Cambridge, pp. 367–388.
Shennan, S.J., Crema, E.R., Kerig, T., 2015. Isolation-by-distance, homophily, and “core”
vs. “package” cultural evolution models in Neolithic Europe. Evol. Hum. Behav. 36,
103–109.
Sjöman, L., 1992. Vasijas de barro: la cerámica popular en el Ecuador. Centro
Interamericano de Artesanías y Artes Populares, Cuenca.
Stark, M.T. (Ed.), 1998. The Archaeology of Social Boundaries, Smithsonian Series in
Archaeological Inquiry. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, WA; London.
Stark, M.T.,
Bishop, R.L., Miska, E., 2000. Ceramic technology and social boundaries:
cultural practices in Kalinga clay selection and use. J. Archaeol. Method Theory 4,
295–332.
Stark, M.T., Bowser, B.J., Horne, L. (Eds.), 2008. Cultural Transmission and Material
Culture. Breaking down Boundaries. The University Arizona Press, Tucson.
Tehrani, J.J., Riede, F., 2008. Towards an archaeology of pedagogy: learning, teaching
and the generation of material culture traditions. World Archaeol. 40, 316–331.
Wiessner, P., 1983. Style and social information in Kalahari San projectile points. Am.
Antiq. 48, 253–276.
Williamson, K., 1971. The Benue-Congo languages and Ijo. Curr. Trends Linguist. 7,
245–306.
Wimmer, A., 2013. Ethnic Boundary Making: Institutions, Power, Networks. Oxford
University Press, Oxford.
V. Roux et al.
Jour nal of Anthropol ogic al Ar chaeol ogy 48 (2017) 320–335
335